The past few weeks have tossed strange prepositional
anomalies in my path. Or perhaps every week does so, but I have just taken to
noticing them recently. The
three-year-old who lives nearby likes to announce, “At my birthday, I’m going
to…”, or “Can I have that at my birthday?” On the classic rock station, I heard
an early 70’s number 1 hit, “A Horse with No Name,” by the group America.
Strangest amalgamated preposition you’ll find: “In the desert you can remember
your name, ‘cause there ain’t no one for to give you no pain”. Forget the
triple negative, it’s that “for to” that leaves you wondering how far the
exigencies of meter should nudge a songwriter toward agrammaticality. On the
street car, I heard a kid announce to his companion, “At least I’m done my
homework!” Shouldn’t there be a “with” in there somewhere? And as we gear up
for the summer Olympics, I expect to be hearing many, many times broadcasters
using the phrase “In an hour from now” (e.g., “We will be poolside for high
diving in an hour from now.”) To my ear, though, “in an hour” all by its
lonesome can hold up just fine the structure that precedes or follows it.
But why should anyone care about prepositions? We
can make ourselves understood in most conversational contexts even if these
small hardly noticeable words are misused, abused, left out, or doubled up. In
his new book, The Secret Life of
Pronouns: What Our Words Say about Us, the social psychologist James W.
Pennebaker demonstrates quite convincingly that we should care about
prepositions and other “function words”. In the preface, he claims, “Pronouns,
articles, prepositions, and a handful of other small, stealthy words reveal
parts of your personality, thinking style, emotional state, and connections
with others. These words… account for less than one-tenth of 1 percent of your
vocabulary but make up almost 60 percent of the words you use” (p. ix). These words are usually very short:
17 of the top 20 function words in English are three letters or shorter, and
they are very hard to learn to use properly in another language after the age
of 12. (And, by my count, 30% of those words as presented in Pennebaker’s table
[p. 26] are prepositions.)
The author presents studies that have discovered fascinating
correlations between high and low usages of particular types of function words
and gender, age, personality, and social status. For example, people who use
more articles (a, an, the,
in English) are likely to be “more organized and emotionally stable”, “more
conscientious, more politically conservative, and older” (p. 37). Women use the
first-person pronoun “I”, cognitive words (such as understand, know, because, reason), and social words more than men, who use more
prepositions and nouns. The use of pronouns most reliably reveals social status.
Those higher in status use much fewer “I-words” (I, me, my), more “we-words” (we, us,
our) and “you-words” (you, your)
(p. 174). Different emotional states are also associated with particular
patterns of function word use: positive emotions are linked to greater use of “we”
and concrete nouns. Sadness is linked to higher rates of saying or writing “I”
and the use of past or future tense verbs. When angry, people use “you,” “he,” “she,”
and “they” more, and rely more on the present tense. Pennebaker also discusses
efforts at lie detection through linguistic analysis, concluding that we should
be skeptical of any analytic process that claims to exhibit greater than about
65% accuracy. However, he does present an interesting table (p. 162) that
classifies several words that tend to be associated either with honesty or
deception. Later chapters examine the language of leadership, of love, and of
group identity, and how computational author identification works. The author also discusses the science of linguistic style matching or
verbal mimicking.
And what of the lowly preposition? Well, it’s not so
lowly, it would seem. Prepositions are associated with “complex thinking”. This
type of thinking, notes the author, involves “bigger words, longer sentences,
and more complicated sentences, often involving prepositions... Prepositions,
by the way are glorious language markers. They help to situate an idea in time
and space” (p. 296). So, the prepositional anomalies I recorded might reveal a few things. The three-year-old's birthday-themed prepositional "at" likely just reveals that, well, she's three years old. It takes children a long time to acquire skillful usage of the prepositions, even in their first language. I'm guessing that the joyful preposition-dropping declaration on the street car probably would tell a member of Pennebaker's research team that the child is a Canadian. And maybe broadcasters subconsciously achieve a bit more status in the minds of the viewers if they shingle more prepositions than are absolutely necessary into their announcements. As for the group America's "for to" excursion, I haven't a clue what to make of that. I'm willing to bet, though, that Pennebaker and his research team would. A little respect then, for the humble to, of, in,
for, with, on that made it onto the top-20 most frequently used words list, and for all those prepositions that didn't. They, and other function words, are telling our secrets.
Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). The Secret Life of Pronouns: What Our Words Say about Us. New York:
Bloomsbury Press.
No comments:
Post a Comment